Meeting Brochure and registration form      SMDM Homepage

Monday, 16 October 2006 - 3:30 PM

UNDERSTANDING PATIENT EXPECTATIONS IN EARLY-PHASE CLINICAL ONCOLOGY TRIALS

Kevin P. Weinfurt, PhD1, Damon M. Seils, MA1, Kate L. Compton, BA1, Daniel P. Sulmasy, OFM, MD, PhD2, Alan B. Astrow, MD3, and Neal J. Meropol, MD4. (1) Duke Clinical Research Institute, Durham, NC, (2) St. Vincent's Hospital Manhattan, New York, NY, (3) Maimonides Medical Center, Brooklyn, NY, (4) Fox Chase Cancer Center, Philadelphia, PA

PURPOSE: Participants in early-phase clinical oncology trials have reported high expectations of personal benefit from their participation. There is concern that this reflects patients' misunderstandings about the trials to which they have consented. Such concerns are based on assumptions about what patients mean when they respond to questions about likelihood of benefit. In this study, we explored some of these assumptions.

METHODS: Participants were 27 women and 18 men enrolled in phase 1 or 2 clinical oncology trials and randomized to 1 of 3 interview protocols corresponding to 3 "target questions" about likelihood of benefit: frequency-type ("Out of 100 patients who participate in this study, how many do you expect will have their cancer controlled as a result of the experimental therapy?"); belief-type ("How confident are you that the experimental therapy will control your cancer?"); and vague ("What is the chance that the experimental therapy will control cancer?"). In semistructured cognitive interviews, we queried participants about how they understood and answered the target question. Each participant then answered and discussed one of the other target questions.

RESULTS: Participants tended to provide higher belief-type expectations (median, 80.0) than frequency-type expectations (median, 50.0) (P=.02). High expectations were expressed even by participants who understood that there was little or no evidence that the experimental therapy would work. The most common justification for responses involved the need for a positive attitude. Other justifications included references to the participant's health and religious faith. It is noteworthy that 27% said they could not answer the target question. The majority (10/12) who answered "don't know" did so for the frequency-type question after first responding to the belief-type question. Participants' reasons for saying "don't know" included that the question is unanswerable, there was not enough information, and the participant did not recall the answer.

CONCLUSIONS: Frequency-type and belief-type questions elicited significantly different responses. Many participants' responses to questions about likelihood of benefit appeared to function as expressions of hope rather than descriptions of the participants' understanding of the clinical trials. This phenomenon makes it challenging to assess patient understanding in this context. Our findings suggest, however, that some patients provide a more candid response to a frequency-type question (ie, "don't know") if first given the opportunity to express confidence in their own outcomes.


See more of Concurrent Abstracts A: Decision Support and Preferences
See more of The 28th Annual Meeting of the Society for Medical Decision Making (October 15-18, 2006)