TRA-2 THE MORAL PSYCHOLOGY OF RATIONING AMONG PHYSICIANS: THE ROLE OF HARM AND FAIRNESS INTUITIONS IN PHYSICIAN OBJECTIONS TO COST-EFFECTIVENESS AND COST-CONTAINMENT

Monday, October 25, 2010: 9:00 AM
Grand Ballroom East (Sheraton Centre Toronto Hotel)
Jon C. Tilburt, MD, MPH1, Katherine M. James, MPH1, Ryan A. Antiel, MA1 and Farr A. Curlin, MD2, (1)Mayo Clinic, Rochester, MN, (2)University of Chicago Medical Center, Chicago, IL

Purpose: We sought to determine if physicians’ personal moral intuitions were associated with their judgments about using cost-effectiveness data in clinical decisions as well as their judgments about cost-containment strategies.

Method: We surveyed 2000 practicing U.S. physicians regarding moral and social issues including their moral intuitions related to five constructs – harm, fairness, ingroup, authority, and purity.  These constructs have been used in social psychology using a 6 point ordinal scale in a 30-item instrument with 5 subscales for each construct.  We hypothesized that harm and fairness subscale ratings would be associated with favorable perceptions of using cost-effectiveness data and cost-containment strategies.  We asked physicians to rate their degree of moral objection (none, moderate, or strong) to using cost-effectiveness data in clinical decisions.  We then asked, on a 4-point scale, to what degree they agreed with limiting reimbursements for expensive drugs and procedures in order to expand coverage to uninsured patients (cost-containment).  We used logistic regression to examine associations between harm and fairness subscale scores and judgments about cost-effectiveness and cost-containment (using dichotomized versions of these items). 

Result: 1032 of 1895 physicians (54%) responded. In unadjusted analyses, harm ratings were significantly associated with moral objection to cost-effectiveness.  For every 1-unit increase in harm subscale scores (0-5), there was a 20% greater chance of objecting to cost-effectiveness analysis in clinical practice (OR= 1.2 [1.0-1.4]). After adjusting for age, gender, region, and specialty, that association was no longer significant. Fairness scores were not associated with judgments about using cost-effectiveness data in clinical practice.  Both harm and fairness were significantly associated with judgments about cost-containment.  For every 1-unit increase in mean harm score, there was a 20% increased chance of agreeing with cost-containment (OR=1.2 [1.0-1.4]).  Similarly, every 1-unit increase in mean fairness scores was associated with a 70% greater chance of agreeing with cost-containment.  These associations were unchanged after adjusting for age, gender, region, and specialty.  There was no association between ingroup, authority and purity and cost-effectiveness or cost-containment judgments.

Conclusion: Differences of opinion among U.S. physicians related to cost-effectiveness data and cost-containment measures may arise from differences in the relative importance they place on key moral intuitions. Efforts to enlist the support of physicians in cost-containment may need to account for this diversity of moral intuitions.